U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Front Psychol

Music Interventions and Child Development: A Critical Review and Further Directions

Elisabeth dumont.

1 Music in Education, Zuyd University of Applied Science, Maastricht, Netherlands

Elena V. Syurina

2 Health, Ethics and Society, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands

3 Faculty of Science, Athena Institute, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, Netherlands

Frans J. M. Feron

4 Social Medicine, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands

Susan van Hooren

5 Healthcare, Zuyd University of Applied Science, Maastricht, Netherlands

Research on the impact of music interventions has indicated positive effects on a variety of skills. These findings suggest musical interventions may have further potential to support educational processes and development of children. This paper reviews the latest evidence on the effect of musical interventions on the development of primary school-aged children. Four electronic databases were searched from January 2010 through June 2016 using music, music instruction, music education, music lesson, music training, development, child, student , and pupil as key words for the search. Two reviewers independently evaluated the studies to determine whether they met the stated inclusion criteria. Studies were compared on study setup, methodological quality, intervention components, outcome variables, and efficacy. A review of these selected studies ( n = 46) suggestive beneficial effects of music intervention on development of children, although clear conclusions cannot be drawn. Possible influencing factors that might contribute to the outcome of intervention are reviewed and recommendations for further research are made.

Introduction

Music interventions are often said to have an influence on motor, language, social, cognitive, and academic abilities (Ho et al., 2003 ; Costa-Giomi, 2004 ; Schellenberg, 2004 ; Forgeard et al., 2008 ; Standley, 2008 ; Jentschke and Koelsch, 2009 ; Southgate and Roscigno, 2009 ; Yazejian and Peisner-Feinberg, 2009 ; Strait et al., 2010 ). Music may play an important role in meeting a child's educational needs as it provides a means of self-expression, giving the child an outlet for feelings and emotions. Music, aside from being a source of enjoyment, is also a means of communication with others (Suthers and Niland, 2007 ). Music may expose the child to challenges and multi-sensory experiences which enhance learning abilities and encourage cognitive development. In particular, music can also engage cognitive functions, such as planning, working memory, inhibition, and flexibility. These functions are known as executive functions (EF). Although there is no consensus on conceptualization, there is agreement on the complexity and the importance of EF for learning and development (Gioia et al., 2000 ). Music education may be a promising tool in improving EF as it activates multiple cortical and subcortical brain areas, including the prefrontal cortex, which is linked to EF (Särkämö et al., 2014 ).

Musical interventions may become an appealing approach for schools that are increasingly facing a challenge of supporting education processes and development of children with varied degrees of learning and behavioral difficulties. However, before an extended use can be introduced into practice, we need to have a clearer, more systematic understanding of the known effects musical interventions have.

The current study builds on the results of previous reviews of literature examining the impact of music training and education including, among others, those of Jaschke et al. ( 2013 ), Cogo-Moreira et al. ( 2012 ), Besson et al. ( 2011 ), Maloy and Peterson ( 2014 ), and Miendlarzewska and Trost ( 2014 ). Jaschke et al. ( 2013 ) found mixed evidence of far transfer effects between music education and other cognitive skills. Cogo-Moreira et al. ( 2012 ) aimed to review RCTs to investigate the effectiveness of music education on reading skills in children and adolescents with dyslexia but were unable to find such studies. In a meta-analysis, Maloy and Peterson ( 2014 ) concluded that there was a minimal effect of music as an intervention to increase task performance in children and adolescents with ADHD. Miendlarzewska and Trost ( 2014 ) found that musical training in childhood has a positive impact on many cognitive functions and is associated with neuroplastic changes in brain structure and function. The transfer of training from music to speech was evaluated by Besson et al. ( 2011 ), who pointed to positive transfer of training effects from musical expertise to speech processing. When interpreting the results, it is important to take into consideration that these reviews in general yielded mixed results and were limited in their focus: specific skills (Cogo-Moreira et al., 2012 ), a specific developmental domain (Miendlarzewska and Trost, 2014 ), specific designs and age groups (Cogo-Moreira et al., 2012 ; Jaschke et al., 2013 ), or a specific target group (Maloy and Peterson, 2014 ).

Bearing these in mind, the purpose of this article is to provide a comprehensive summary of the existing research in the field by collecting and analyzing the latest evidence on the effect of music interventions across different domains of development of the primary school-aged children. It aims to report on the effectiveness of a broad range of music interventions, describe relevant contextual factors, to evaluate the general level and quality of evidence in the field and to provide implications for future research.

Due to a broad scope of this study, we decided to do a systematic search and a “critical review,” which aims to “extensively research the literature and critically evaluate its quality” (Grant and Booth, 2009 ). Several steps were taken in order to ensure high scientific quality of the work.

Search procedure

The search for relevant articles was conducted via three routes. First, PubMed, EMBASE (Ovid), PsycInfo, and EBESCO databases were systematically searched. The search covered 6 years (January 2010 to June 2016) and the following search terms were included: music, music education, music instruction, music lesson, music training, development, child * , student, pupil . The key-words were combined in various ways using Boolean terms AND and OR. Second, reference lists of the identified relevant systematic reviews and key articles (referenced by more than 1 paper) were examined in order to identify additional studies. The last route included a manual search of the tables of contents of relevant journals: International Journal of Music Education and British Journal of Music Education. A flowchart describing these processes is reported in Figure ​ Figure1 1 .

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-08-01694-g0001.jpg

Flow diagram of article identification and inclusion.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Identified studies were considered eligible for inclusion if they met the following a priori defined criteria. The studies had to (a) involve training, teaching, or providing intervention using music; (b) utilize outcome measures targeting child's development; (c) focus on the (pre)school-aged children up to 13 years without physical disabilities; (d) be published in a peer-reviewed journal between January 2010 and June 2016; (e) be written in English. We excluded studies that (a) examined use of music psychotherapy interventions; (b) focused only on imaging techniques, (c) had musical outcomes only; (d) were not based on empirical data: qualitative reviews, commentaries, case studies or studies without an accurate methodological description.

Screening and study selection

Upon removal of the duplicates, the literature search yielded 1,092 results. All identified studies were subjected to multilevel screening, executed independently by two co-authors (ED and EVS). First, the titles and abstracts of identified studies were screened. At this stage, the titles and abstracts that did not meet at least one inclusion criteria (non-English language, commentaries) were omitted. Based on this first screening, 126 potentially relevant articles were obtained as full texts. Next, these articles were further reviewed by ED and EVS independently to determine whether or not they met the stated inclusion criteria. All exclusion decisions were documented. Each reviewer made a selection list, which were then compared. In cases of disagreement, the articles in question were discussed by all co-authors and a consensus decision was made. Our final selection included 46 articles.

Data analysis

Studies that met at least one inclusion criteria, but did not meet any of the exclusion criteria, have been reported according to a list of five variables in order to extract data in a comparable way. The methodological quality of the studies was assessed using the guidelines of the Dutch Institute for Health Care Improvement (CBO). The following elements were evaluated: randomization, allocation concealment, baseline comparability, blinding of participants or providers, blinding of outcome assessors, reporting of attrition rate, the use of intent-to-treat analyses and the use of validated tools. The level of evidence of each study was determined according to the guidelines of Melnyk and Fineout-Overholt ( 2005 ).

The main results of selected studies are reported in Table ​ Table1. 1 . All studies involved participants of 4–13 years-old, but some were not limited to this range: two studies chose a broad age range of 6–25 years [35] and 6–59 years [32]. Sample sizes varied between 10 [1] and 352 [35]. In general, studies employed both genders. One study [40] and one sub-experiment of a study [42] included males only. Although the type of design was not explicitly mentioned in all studies, most studies implemented a(n) (quasi-) experimental, longitudinal, or correlational design. Only three of the 46 studies used a randomized control trial (RCT) [21] [29] [17].

Intervention details for included studies.

The reviewed articles have spawned a broad range of approaches to and considerable heterogeneity in music interventions. In general, music interventions consisted either of structured musical instruction/activities, i.e., use of instruments, singing, moving, listening, improvising, music notation, rhythm training, composing music, instrumental classes, or private instrumental training. Only in several studies, the music intervention was especially designed for the acquisition of specific non-musical skills [10] [11]. Length of the intervention varied across studies, ranging from seven and a half minutes [31] to 11 semesters [43]. Music interventions were mostly provided two or three times per week. In three studies, interventions were delivered on a daily basis [15] [16] [25]. All but two studies [32] [40] used live music as opposed to recorded music. Four studies used a specific pedagogical approach for music instruction: the Orff method, which refers to a way of teaching children about music that engages their mind and body through a mixture of singing, dancing, acting, and the use of percussion instruments or the Kodaly method, in which children are first introduced to concepts of music through experiences such as singing, listening or movement. Only after the child becomes familiar with the concept of music do they learn how to compose it [8] [15] [28] [45]. Interventions were either performed in (small) groups or individual (in case of instrumental training). The authors conducted studies either in the school/classroom environment, where music interventions would be regularly conducted; or used locations outside school i.e., music schools or specific center for music teaching. Information about the person who delivered the music intervention was mentioned in 30 studies. In most studies a professionally trained music teacher was employed. In two other studies, the intervention was delivered by parents/teachers [4] who received training or by trained research assistants [11]. In four studies, the intervention was either computer-based [16] [25] or delivered via CD/radio [32] [40].

The reviewed articles used varied outcome measures affected by music interventions. Outcome measures can be grouped in the following categories: motor skill development, social and emotional development, language, cognitive development, academic performance and other, non-musical, related skills.

Motor skill development

We identified no studies that focused particularly on the association between music training and gross motor skill development. Two studies explored, among others, the beneficial impact of music activity on specific motor skills. Using a non-randomized design, Brodsky and Sulkin ( 2011 ) [1] (which presented results of three experiments) focused on hand-clapping songs. In the first experiment, the association of performance quality of handclapping songs with academic achievement was evaluated among a class of 18 children (mean age 7 years). Two handclapping songs were taught by rote via live demonstration by the second author during a 3-week period and both performance quality and achievement of all 18 children were assessed. Results indicated that children who were more skillful in performing handclapping songs, were also more efficient learners. In a second experiment, the authors measured bimanual rhythmic patting and aural diction in 10 children aged 8–8.5 years, five children who self-reported engagement in handclapping songs activity, and five children from the same classroom who self-reported not to engage in handclapping songs were recruited. Self-reports were confirmed by the second author through observations. The authors found that children who spontaneously engaged in hand clapping songs had an advantage in aural diction and accuracy performance of eye-hand motor sequences. The third experiment took place over 8 weeks. Twenty-four children received classroom handclapping intervention (HCST) while another 27 received the music appreciation guided listening curriculum (MAGL). Children who received HCST were more effective in developing bimanual coupling, writing proficiencies and handwriting compared to children who received MAGL (Brodsky and Sulkin, 2011 [1]). Janzen et al. ( 2014 ) [2] investigated whether formal music training enhances precision in discrete and continuous movements. The study included 32 children enrolled in music classes who had at least 2 h of weekly musical activities. Twenty-five children who were not involved in any musical activity were also included. All were 10–14 years-old. Results showed that musically trained children had a significantly more accurate performance in the discrete movement task compared to controls. Findings suggest performance was positively associated with the number of years of formal music training. Musically trained children also tended to be more precise in the continuous movement task (Janzen et al., 2014 ).

Although reporting positive results, a limitation of above-mentioned, quasi-experimental studies was the lack of randomization. In a sub-experiment of one study only (sub experiment 3) [1], participants were matched socioeconomically and an active control group was included. In the second study of another sub-experiment, performance of the music group was compared to control groups who were not involved in music training [2] or who did not receive any additional activity (sub experiment 2) [1]. Therefore, caution should be used when making inferences about the observed effects of the music interventions on specific motor skills.

Social and emotional development

Social skills.

Four studies reported mixed evidence of the influence of music interventions on social skills. Using a quasi-experimental design, Ritblatt et al. ( 2013 ) [4] found that 55 children, aged 3–5, who received a music intervention program focused on socioemotional skills, demonstrated a positive change in these skills compared to a wait-list control group ( n = 47) who did not receive the music intervention. These changes occurred over the course of a 8-month period. It's important to note that these effects were reported by teachers and not parents. Schellenberg et al. ( 2015 ) [5] investigated whether social benefits were accrued from an existing group music training program that was designed with music pedagogy as its focus in 84 8–9 year-old children. Results showed that children in the music group ( n = 38), who attended schools that incorporated an enhanced group music program into the curriculum, had larger increases in sympathy and prosocial behavior compared to children in the control group ( n = 46), who attended schools without the enhanced music program, but this effect was limited to children who had poor prosocial skills before the lessons began. Evidence from a between-participants study of the effects of joint music making in 48 pairs of 4 year-old children [3] who were randomly assigned either to the music condition (i.e., episode of interactive play with joint music making) or the non-music condition (i.e., episode of interactive play without music), demonstrated an increase in willingness to help one another and to cooperate on a problem-solving task in children in the music condition compared to non-music condition (Kirschner and Tomasello, 2010 ) [3]. However, in an experimental study, Rickard et al. ( 2013 ) [8] assigned 195 5–8 year-old children to either a music education ( n = 122) or a control group ( n = 73) based on the school they were attending. Children receiving a music education received age-specific, specialized music programs on top of the preexisting, general school music program, while children in the control group did not receive these specialized music programs but continued with their regular school music program. The authors found no benefits of the specialized music program on children's social skills compared to children in the control group.

In sum, three studies [3] [4] [5] reported partially positive results, whereas one study [8] reported no effects. One study reporting a beneficial impact of music [3] is of high quality$ i.c. incorporating random assignment to conditions, blinding the outcome assessors and incorporating an active, matched control program without music. The intervention lasted, however, for 20 min. The partially positive findings of Ritblatt et al. ( 2013 ) [4] and Schellenberg et al. ( 2015 ) [5] should be interpreted with caution due to the design used [5], the lack of randomization, the fact the sample may not be representative of the target population (i.c. higher SES and higher educational level) [4] and teacher/parent expectations which may have influenced the results [4]. In the experimental study [8] of Rickard et al. ( 2012 ) [8], reporting no effects of a specialized music program on top of the general school music program, randomization was absent. However, the relatively large sample size, the duration of the study and the inclusion of an active control group are strengths of this study.

Results of above mentioned studies are mixed and demonstrate the need for further research.

Emotional development

Two studies addressed the influence of music on emotional development and reported mixed results. A study of Schellenberg and Mankarious ( 2012 ) [7] assessed 60 children, ranging from 7 to 8 years-old, on a test of emotion comprehension (TEC). The musically trained group included 30 children who had at least 8 months of formal music lessons taken outside the school, whereas the untrained group consisted of 30 children who had no music training outside the school. Musically trained children demonstrated significantly higher TEC scores than the ones without music training. The effect remained even after accounting for demographic variables. However, the link appeared to be a consequence of high level cognitive functioning of the musically trained group. No group differences were present when IQ scores were accounted for. Using an experimental study, Rabinowitch et al. ( 2013 ) [6] tracked 52 children aged 8–11 after they were randomly assigned to either a musical group interaction program ( n = 23), a games group ( n = 8), receiving a similar program without the use of music or a control group ( n = 21), not receiving any special activity. Children in the music group showed an increase in empathy scores on two out of three measures compared to children in the games group and children in the control group.

While both studies reported positive results, the findings of the study of Schellenberg and Mankarious ( 2012 ) [7] turned out to be a related to the level of cognitive functioning of participants in the music group. The experimental study of Rabinowitch et al. ( 2013 ) [6] permits, at least to some extent, for causal inference. The authors used randomization to allocate participants to conditions, thereby reducing the risk of bias from confounding. The small sample size and the fact that the active and the passive control group were merged into one control group before comparison with the music group, should, however, be taken into consideration. Based on findings from both studies, no definitive conclusions can be drawn yet and more research is needed in this area to achieve conclusive results.

Academic self-concept, psychosocial wellbeing, and self-esteem

Three studies reported mixed effects of music on academic self-concept, which refers to the cognitive representation and appraisal of one's own abilities in academic performance (Degé et al., 2014 ), psychosocial wellbeing and self-esteem, which describes one's overall sense of self-worth. In a correlational study, Degé et al. ( 2014 ) [9] revealed that duration of music lessons was positively associated with academic self-concept in 92 12–14 year-old children, even after controlling for demographic variables and IQ. In a 3 year experimental study, Rickard et al. ( 2013 ) [8] showed that increase in school-based music lessons prevented a decline in global self-esteem measures experienced by the control group in both the younger and older cohorts across the first year of the study. However, effect sizes were generally modest in the second year. In another study, Rickard et al. ( 2012 ) [42] investigated the effect of increasing existing music education (study 1) and the effect of introducing a novel high-quality music education program (study two) on various psychosocial measures in 111 10–13 year-old children (all males). One hundred eleven 10–13 year-old children in study one were pseudo-randomly assigned to additional music classes ( n = 47), art classes ( n = 27), or drama classes ( n = 37). One hundred six children in study two (mean age 131.07 months) were randomly allocated to a music group ( n = 38), a drama group ( n = 37) or control group, receiving no program ( n = 31). No significant effects were found.

Degé et al. ( 2014 ) [9], using a correlational design, was the only one reporting positive results. However, these results do not allow for any conclusions to be drawn about causality. Two experimental studies of Rickard et al. ( 2012 ) [8] [42] found modest effects and no effects, respectively music interventions on top of the preexisting school music education. The (relatively) large sample sizes [8] [42] and the duration of one the studies [8] can be considered as strengths. Both studies, did not, however, randomize participants to the intervention or control groups. In one of the two experimental studies [42], active control groups were included, who continued their regular school music program. The other study [8] included both passive and active control groups, which better allowed for comparison of the increased music education.

In summary, although one study reported positive correlations, two studies suggest little or no beneficial effect. Further research is needed to clarify whether music can positively impact self-concept, self-esteem, and psychosocial wellbeing.

Studies that link music intervention to language acquisition can be clustered into two groups: (1) focus on phonological awareness and auditory processing and (2) reading.

Phonological awareness and auditory processing

Several studies assessed the influence of music on auditory and phonological skills with mixed findings. Some suggest that musical activities have a beneficial effect on these skills. Using a descriptive-comparative design, Escalda et al. ( 2011 ) [12] examined the relationship between musical experience, auditory processing abilities and phonological awareness skills of 56 five year-old children. Results showed that 26 children, with musical experience, performed significantly better on auditory processing and phonological awareness than 30 children without musical experience. In an exploratory study, Moritz et al. ( 2013 ) [15] investigated whether musical activity could enhance the acquisition of reading skill, potentially before formal reading instructions began in 30 children (mean age 5.6 years). Children in the music group ( n = 15) received daily 45 min music lessons whereas children in the control group ( n = 15) received weekly 35-min music lessons. Correlational results showed that rhythm ability was related to phonological segmentation skills at the beginning of kindergarten and that end-of-year phonological awareness skills of children who received daily music lessons were better than skills of children in the control group who received music lessons once a week. Using a pragmatic RCT, Cogo-Moreira et al. ( 2013 ) [21] included 235 participants with reading problems, aged 8–10 years, in 10 schools, to compare the effectiveness of music education for the improvement of among other, reading skills. Five schools were randomly chosen to incorporate music classes ( n = 114) and five schools, who were not encouraged to offer musical activities, served as controls ( n = 121). There was no improvement in phonological awareness when comparing the two groups. Flaugnacco et al. ( 2015 ) [17], also using an RCT, pseudo-randomly assigned 8–11 year-old dyslexic children to a music group ( n = 24) or a painting group ( n = 24). Both groups also received conventional rehabilitation program. After 7 months of training, the music group outperformed the painting group in tasks assessing rhythmic abilities and phonological awareness. Using a pretest/training/posttest design, Moreno et al. ( 2011b ) [16] focused on the effects of an intensive computerized training in music or visual arts on pre-literacy skills in 60 4–6 year-old children, who were pseudo-randomly assigned to the music or visual arts condition. They reported comparable improvements in both groups in rhyme awareness and in ability to map unfamiliar symbols to known words. However, when the two groups were statistically equated at pretest, the magnitude of improvement was found to be larger in the music group. Herrera et al. ( 2011 ) [13] on the other hand, used a 2 year pretest-posttest study in which 97 children (mean age 4.5 years) at two preschools were allocated following stratified randomization procedures into a group that received phonological training with music ( n = 32), a group that received phonological training with no music ( n = 34) and a control group who did not receive any specialized training ( n = 31). Phonological training was effective regardless of whether it included music and whether the children were foreign Spanish speakers or native speakers. Both experimental treatment groups outperformed the control group in the posttests on phonological awareness tasks and speed in naming objects. However, the phonological training with music group outperformed the phonological training without music group on phonological awareness of ending sounds. In general, the foreign Spanish speakers were significantly slower in the naming task than their Spanish counterparts, those who had participated in the training with musical activities outperformed their peers in the control group by the end of the treatment. Bhide et al. ( 2013 ) [10] compared the effects of a musical intervention for poor readers ( n = 10) with a software intervention of known beneficial effects based on rhyme training and phoneme-grapheme learning ( n = 9) in 6–7 year-old children, all of them identified by their class teachers as struggling readers. The authors found that both interventions were equally effective for literacy acquisition and phonological skills. Habib et al. ( 2016 ) [18] examined the effectiveness of a specially-designed Cognitivo-musical training (CMT) in two studies. In study one, 12 children with a diagnosis of severe dyslexia (mean age 10.7 years) received daily 6 h of CMT on 3 consecutive days while 22 reading-age matched normal-reading children (30 months younger on average) served as controls, receiving no CMT. The authors found that dyslexic children were impaired in the identification test of categorical perception, but their performance reached the level of control children after 3 days of CMT. Significant improvement in performance of dyslexic children was also noticed in the syllabic lengthening task. In study two, 12 dyslexic children, grouped according to the severity of their problems received CMT training sessions at school. The 3-h weekly sessions were provided over a period of 6 weeks. Results showed a positive influence of the CMT program on categorical perception and the temporal aspects of speech processing. Also, additional improvements in auditory attention, phonological awareness (syllable fusion) were found. Fonseca-Mora et al. ( 2015 ) [14], using a pre-post comparison design, tested the efficacy of a phonological training program aimed at improving early reading skills in 7–8 year-old Spanish children learning English as a foreign language in three groups: an experimental group with phonological non-musical intervention ( n = 22), an experimental group with musical intervention ( n = 18) and a control group receiving the traditional teaching program ( n = 23). The results clearly pointed to the beneficial effects of the phonological teaching approach, but the further impact of the music support was not demonstrated. In a longitudinal, experimental study, Degé and Schwarzer ( 2011 ) [11] investigated the effect of a music program on phonological awareness in preschoolers. Forty-one children (mean age 5.6 years) were randomly assigned to a music program ( n = 13), a phonological skills program ( n = 13), or a sport group ( n = 14). Results indicated that 26 children who followed either the music program or the phonological program significantly improved in phonological awareness of large phonological units (words) compared to the sport group who received no intervention. All three groups showed similar development in phonological awareness of small phonological units.

Of the two RCTs reviewed, one found beneficial effects of a music intervention on phonological awareness [17] while the other found no effects [21]. Two experimental studies [11] [13], using (stratified) randomization and including two active control groups [11] and an active and passive control group [13], respectively, reported no beneficial effects of music. Of the four studies that used a quasi-experimental design without randomization, two studies, including an active [10] or both an active and passive control group [14], also found no benefits of music interventions One out of these four studies, reporting positive results, used pseudo-random allocation, included an active control group and blinded outcome assessors [16]. Another study, also describing positive results, matched participants, but did include a passive control group only [18]. Positive results were also reported by two correlational studies [12] [15]. However, these results do not allow for any conclusions to be drawn about causality. Although findings suggest music can positively affect phonological awareness and auditory processing in some situations, clear conclusions cannot be drawn.

Eight studies addressed the association between music-related activities and a range of reading skills with inconsistent findings. The results of the study of Cogo-Moreira et al. ( 2013 ) [21] indicated no improvement in word accuracy, in-text accuracy and non-word accuracy of children in the music intervention schools compared to the children in control ones. In contrast, the RCT results of Flaugnacco et al. ( 2015 ) [17] showed better performance of the music group on reading skills in comparison to the control group. Using an experimental design, Bonacina et al. ( 2015 ) [20] randomly assigned 11–14 year-old children to a computer-assisted, rhythmic reading training (RRT) ( n = 14) or a control group ( n = 14), for which no specific activity addressed to improve reading skills was carried out. Results indicated that RRT had a positive effect on both reading speed and accuracy. The effect of RRT seemed to be specifically on reading skills, as no difference in rhythm perception between the two groups was found. Moritz et al. ( 2013 ) [15] found that kindergarteners' rhythm ability was significantly correlated to their phonological awareness and basic word identification skills in second grade. Using a longitudinal design, Slater et al. ( 2014 ) [22] compared reading ability of 42 low-income, Spanish-English bilingual children aged 6-to-9, pseudo-randomly assigned to a group music instruction program outside school or a waiting list control group. Twenty-three children in the music group maintained their age-normed performance on the composite reading measure after 1 year, whereas the performance of 19 children in the matched control group deteriorated over the same period of time, consistent with expected declines in this population. Rautenberg ( 2013 ) [23], in an experimental study, measured the correlations between musical skills and decoding skills and the effects of musical training on word-level reading abilities. One hundred fifty-nine seven year-old children were randomly allocated to a special music training program ( n = 33), a visual arts training program ( n = 41), or no training program for the period of the study ( n = 85). Results showed the special music training had a significant effect on reading accuracy in word reading. Additionally, positive correlations were found between rhythmical ability and decoding skills. Tonal skills were not correlated with reading skills. In a correlational study of Corrigall and Trainor ( 2011 ) [19], it was shown that duration of music training (i.e., the number of years of training on their primary instrument, plus the number of years of training on any additional instruments) was associated with reading comprehension, but not with word decoding among 46 6–9 year-olds. The findings are in contrast to a longitudinal study from Bergman Nutley et al. ( 2014 ) [35] which revealed that practicing a musical instrument was not associated with reading comprehension.

Of the eight studies measuring the effects on reading, two studies used an RCT design with pseudo randomization [17] [21] and blinded outcome assessors [17]. Their findings are contradictory; Flaugnacco et al. ( 2015 ) [17] found a positive influence of music, whereas the results of Cogo-Moreira et al. ( 2013 ) [21] indicated no effect. Results of two studies that used an experimental design with randomization [20] [23] illustrated potential benefits of a music training program. Of these two studies, one included a passive control group, offering no music training program [20] while the other included both a passive and active control group [23], allowing for a more comprehensive comparison. The results of the longitudinal study of Slater et al. ( 2015 ) [46] also point to beneficial effects. However, an active control group could not be included. (Partially) positive correlations were shown by two studies [15] [19]. However, correlational studies do not allow for causal inferences. In another longitudinal study [35], participants were compared to themselves. Attrition rate and practice effects might, however, have influenced the results.

As results of above-mentioned studies are both positive and negative, findings in this area are inconclusive.

Cognitive development

In this review, studies focusing on the effects of music on children's cognitive abilities were subdivided into three categories, reflecting different aspects of cognitive development: (1) intelligence, (2) memory, and (3) attention and other executive function skills.

Intelligence

Several studies have explored the effects of music intervention on intelligence. Results from these studies suggest little or no beneficial effects. In an experimental design, Kaviani et al. ( 2014 ) [28] randomly allocated 60 5–6 year-old children to two groups, the experimental group receiving Orff music lessons and the other (matched for age-, sex-, and mother's educational level) receiving no lessons The authors demonstrated that after participating in the Orff music program for 3 months, children had significantly higher scores on the visual abstract reasoning, verbal reasoning and short term memory subscales of the Stanford—Binet Intelligence Scale compared to children, who did not receive any musical lessons. Schellenberg ( 2011 ) [24] and Bergman Nutley et al. ( 2014 ) [35] also reported positive associations between, respectively, music training and IQ and music training and non-verbal reasoning. In a longitudinal study, Moreno et al. ( 2011a ) [25] used two subtests of the WPSI III (vocabulary and block design) to examine the influence of two interactive computerized training programs (music and visual arts) on, among other skills, verbal and spatial intelligence in 64 4–6 year-old children who were pseudo-randomly allocated to one of the two conditions. They found that children who participated in a computerized music training program showed enhanced performance on the measure of vocabulary knowledge. Not in line with above mentioned findings is the study of Mehr et al. ( 2013 ) [29]. They conducted an RCT to investigate the effects of parent-child music education on specific cognitive skills in preschool children. In experiment one, four-year-old children were randomly assigned to a music group ( n = 15) or a visual arts group ( n = 14). In experiment two, 23 children were randomly allocated to a music group and 22 children to a control group who did not receive music classes. Analyses with a combined music group ( n = 38), the visual arts group and the control group revealed no significant effects on spatial-navigational reasoning, visual form analysis, numerical discrimination, and receptive vocabulary. Rickard et al. ( 2012 ) [42] failed to find an effect of increased classroom based music education on various cognitive measures. Bugos and Jacobs ( 2012 ) [26] evaluated the effects of a composition program, Composers in Public Schools (CiPS), on cognitive skills among 28 sixth-graders who were assigned to an experimental group ( n = 15), receiving the CiPS program or a control group ( n = 13), not participating in any musical courses. Results showed enhanced performance in arithmetic scores of the WISC-IV for the experimental group compared to the control group. No effect was found for vocabulary performance. Due to a relatively large variation in scores, enhancements for digit coding and symbol search subtests were not significant.

Only one out of the seven studies measuring the effects of music on intelligence, employed an RCT design including an active as well as a passive control group [29], which permits causal inferences to be made. No significant effects were found in that particular study. The two experimental studies reviewed [28] [42] yielded mixed results. While both used randomization [28] [sub experiment 2, 42], only one study, reporting no effect, included an active control group [42]. The remaining four studies, employed a quasi-experimental (longitudinal) design [24] [25] [26] or longitudinal developmental design [35], showed positive or partially positive effects. However, only one out of these four studies used pseudo-randomized group assignment, blinded outcome assessors and included an active control group. Schellenberg ( 2011 ) [24] and Bugos and Jacobs ( 2012 ) [26] both included a passive control group. Neither study matched participants on baseline variables. Despite the large sample size and duration of the study, caution is needed in interpreting findings of Bergman Nutley et al. ( 2014 ) [35], due to attrition and the possible influence of practice effects.

A number of studies looked specifically at aspects of memory with mixed results. Degé et al. ( 2011 ) [31] demonstrated in a non-randomized, longitudinal design that after 2 years of extended music curriculum (ECM) training, short-term visual, and auditory memory scores for 16 9–11 year-old children, attending ECM training, had improved significantly, whereas no such increase was found in 25 children who did not attend ECM training. Roden et al. ( 2012 ) [33] conducted a quasi-experimental study where participants were allocated to a music program, a science program or a control group. Results showed that 25 children (mean age 7.73 years), who took part in a school-based music program, outperformed 25 children receiving extended natural science training and 23 children in a control group receiving no additional training, on verbal memory tasks. The authors failed to show a link between type of program and visual memory. Brodsky and Sulkin ( 2011 ) [1] reported positive effects of classroom handclapping intervention (HCST) on verbal memory. Results of a longitudinal study by Rickard et al. ( 2010 ) [30] showed significant enhancement of verbal learning and immediate verbal recall scores in 82 children (mean age 8.62 years) after ~1 year, but not 2 years after non-random allocation to an increased classroom-based instrumental music training, compared to 68 children (mean age 8.79 years), who did not receive training. In an experimental design, Martens et al. ( 2011 ) [32] focused on the effect of musical experience on verbal memory in 38 individuals with Williams syndrome, aged 6–59 years. Participants who had participated in formal music lessons scored significantly better on a verbal long-term memory task when the stimuli were sung than when they were spoken in comparison to those who did not have formal lessons, showing no benefit for either sung or spoken condition. Short-term memory did not appear to be affected by musical experience.

The five studies reviewed yielded mixed results. One experimental study [32] showed improved performance of participants who had participated in formal music lessons. However, generalizability of findings is low by including only participants with Williams syndrome, making the participants a non-representative sample. The remaining four studies, reporting positive or partially positive results, employed quasi-experimental (longitudinal) designs [1] [30] [31] [33]. However, in none of these four studies, participants were randomized or matched on potentially influencing variables, decreasing validity of findings. Blinded outcome assessors were used in one study [30]. Two out of the four studies included an active control group [1] or both an active and passive control group [33], allowing for a more detailed comparison. Although studies suggest potential benefits, the methodological limitations do not allow clear conclusions to be drawn about the effect of music and the part(s) of memory of which music can have an effect on.

Attention and other EF skills

The impact of music interventions on attention and several executive function skills was reported in seven studies with mixed evidence. One study of 102 7–12 year-olds Khalil et al. ( 2013 ) [37] found that, those, who were able to synchronize to a driving beat (in the context of a music class), were more attentive, showed less ADHD-like behaviors (rated by teachers) and performed better on an attention control task, in comparison to those who were less capable of synchronizing. Positive results have also been shown by Moreno et al. ( 2011a ) [25], who reported enhanced performance on accuracy on a go/no-go task. Using a cross-sectional design, Zuk et al. ( 2014 ) [38] assessed (among other participants) 27 children (mean age 10 years) on a range of EF tasks. Fifteen instrumentally trained children, who started training on average at 5 years and had been studying their instruments on average 5.2 years, demonstrated heightened performance on coding, cognitive flexibility and processing speed tasks in comparison with 12 children without musical training outside the requirements of the general music curriculum in school. In contrast, Roden et al. ( 2014 ) [27], using a quasi-experimental design, investigated, among other skills, the effects of music lessons on processing speed abilities and visual attention in 7–8 year-old children over a period of 18 months. In the study, 345 children were assigned to the music training group ( n = 192) or the natural science training group ( n = 153). Children in the music group showed significant increases in information processing speed from T2 to T3. However, the level of significance was only associated with a small effect size. Although both groups improved their visual attention scores over time, these increases were stronger from T1 to T2 and T2 to T3 in children with natural science training as compared to children with music training. In a quasi-experimental study, Schellenberg ( 2011 ) [24] found that, with the exception of digit span, music training was independent of performance on phonological fluency, inhibition, problem solving, and planning and mental flexibility and rule switching. Bugos and Jacobs ( 2012 ) [26] found no effect of participation in a 4-month composition program on verbal fluency. Using an intervention design, Janus et al. ( 2016 ) [39] pseudo-randomly assigned 57 4–6 year-old children (matched on age and cognitive scores) to a 20-day music training ( n = 28) or conversational French training program ( n = 29) to compare the effects on executive control abilities. The one training-specific outcome found was that children in the French group showed broader improvement in visual search than children in the music program. For verbal fluency, grammatical judgement and visual search, all children performed significantly better after training.

Several studies suggested music training may improve various aspects of working memory. In one quasi-experimental, longitudinal study (Roden et al., 2014 ) [34], examined working memory performance in 25 7–10 year-old children who participated in a classroom-based, extended instrumental music training program and 25 children who participated in an extended science training program. Results showed significant gains in two out of three components of working memory performance in children who followed the music program for one-and-a-half-years in comparison to children who took part in the science training group. Positive associations between musical practice and working memory were also reported by Bergman Nutley et al. ( 2014 ) [35] and Zuk et al. ( 2014 ) [38]. Portowitz et al. ( 2014 ) [36] reported a significant enhancement in working memory scores in 62 9–10 year-old children after a 4-month participation in the (computerized) In Harmony program compared to 22 controls who did not participate in this program. The results of the study of Janus et al. ( 2016 ) [39] showed no effect of a music training program on spatial working memory.

The seven studies reviewed yielded mixed results of the influence of music interventions on attention and other EF skills. Positive correlations were shown by one study [37]. However, correlational studies do not allow for causal inferences. The remaining six studies were quasi-experimental (longitudinal) without randomization [24] [25] [26] [27] [38] [39]. Two of these six studies, reporting positive results, used pseudo random allocation of participants to groups [25] or matched participants on potentially confounding variables [38] but only one included an active control group [25]. Two other studies reported mixed and modest results, respectively [27] [24]. The sample size and the inclusion of an active control group can be considered as strength of one of them [27]. Of the remaining two studies [26] [39], reporting no evidence of beneficial effects of music, only one used blinded outcome assessors, pseudo randomization, and included an active control group [39]. Regarding working memory, there seems to be a hint of a positive influence of music based on the results of five studies [34] [35] [36] [38] [39]. However, studies were quasi-experimental (longitudinal) without randomization [34] [36] [38] [39] or longitudinal developmental [35]. Only three out of the five used pseudo random allocation [39] or matched participants on potentially confounding variables [36] [38]. An active control group was included by two out these five studies [39] [34], reporting no effects and mixed effects, respectively. The three other studies, all reporting positive findings, included a passive control group [36] [38] or compared the participants to themselves [35].

Although part of the evidence points to potential benefits, more research is needed to determine whether music can positively impact these skills.

Academic performance

Studies exploring the effect of music on academic performance were subdivide into four categories: (1) school readiness, (2) classroom behavior and academic skills and (3) language, and (4) mathematics.

School readiness

One study focused, among other skills, on preschool children's school-readiness skills. The results of a quasi-experimental study of Ritblatt et al. ( 2013 ) [4] showed that participation in a music program had a positive effect on promoting a positive approach to learning. No effect was found for promoting academic skills.

Methodological limitations of this study are the lack over control over assignments of participants to conditions and the fact the sample may not be representative of the population as whole (i.c. higher SES and higher educational level), creating threats to validity. Information about the blinding of outcome assessors was not provided. The intervention was provided by trained teachers and parents, whose expectations may have influenced outcomes. Taking the limitations into account and the fact that the findings are based on one study only, accuracy and direction of the results should be interpreted with caution.

Classroom behavior and academic skills

There is no evidence that music can affect classroom behavior and academic skills. Pelham et al. ( 2011 ) [40] followed up 41 boys with ADHD and 26 normal comparison boys, who had never been referred for treatment of behavior problems (mean age 9 years) to examine the effects of music and video on classroom behavior and performance. Three distractor conditions (music, video, no-distractor) were randomly introduced for 24 days, varying on a daily basis (8 days in each distractor condition). Neither boys with ADHD or the control group were significantly distracted by music. Within the ADHD group, there were, however, considerable differences in response to the music such that some were adversely affected and others benefited relative to no-distractor. This study included males only, thereby eliminating a potential source of variability. Except for gender, participants were, however, not matched on any other variable. Outcome assessors were not blinded and the distractor conditions and no-distractor conditions may have been not much different. The accuracy and direction of the results should be interpreted with caution as findings are based on one study only.

Several studies have explored the association between a music intervention or music training and performance on (specific) language skills respectively, with contradictory findings. With regard to first language skills, results of an RCT by Cogo-Moreira et al. ( 2013 ) [21] showed positive growing slopes in Portuguese language in the children who completed a 5-month music education program in comparison to the control children. Findings were in contrast to the results of Yang et al. ( 2014 ) [43] who, using a non-randomized, longitudinal design, examined the relation between long-term music training and, among other skills, academic development of Chinese language among 250 Chinese elementary school students (mean age 78 months). Children who took part in formal music training out of school around the beginning of semester three, were categorized as musician children ( n = 77) whereas the remaining children, who had not received formal music training throughout this study, were categorized as non-musician children ( n = 173). Music training was not related to the enhancement of performance on Chinese language.

Regarding second language abilities, Swaminathan and Gopinath ( 2013 ) [44] explored second-language abilities of musically trained children ( n = 37)(mean age 100.55 months), who reported at least 3 months of music training and speaking a language other than English at home, and untrained children ( n = 39)(mean age 98.89 months) and found that the musically trained children (mean length of training 17.63 months) performed significantly better on the tests of comprehension and vocabulary compared to their untrained counterparts. The advantage persisted even when the trained group only consisted of participants trained in Indian Classical music, indicating that the English L2 advantage was not merely because of an increased opportunity to learn new words from songs as Indian Classical music is written in Indian languages. Positive findings were also reported by Yang et al. ( 2014 ) [43], who found that musician children outperformed non-musician children on second language development.

Two studies [21] [43] reported contradictory results on the potential benefit of music on first language development. However, only findings from Cogo-Moreira et al. ( 2013 ) [21], conducting an RCT, allow for conclusions to be drawn about causality. Although the duration and sample size of Yang et al. ( 2014 ) [43] can be considered as strengths, participants were not randomized and a passive control was included. Another two studies reported positive results on second language development [43] [44]. Both studies made a comparison of the music group with a control group, who had no previous musical training. However, only one study [44] controlled for several baseline variables and used blinded outcome assessors, thereby increasing the validity of their findings.

Mathematics

Four studies have explored the effects of music on mathematics. A longitudinal study of Bergman Nutley et al. ( 2014 ) [35] yielded a positive association between music training (i.e., the number of hours per week of practice on instruments played) and performance on mathematics. Cogo-Moreira et al. ( 2013 ) [21] also observed positive growing slopes in math grades. In Yang et al's ( 2014 ) [43] study, however, music training was not related to performance on mathematics. Courey et al. ( 2012 ) [41] examined the efficacy of a music intervention aimed to teach fractions to third graders using a quasi-experimental design. Sixty-seven 8–11 year-olds were assigned by class to either a 6-week academic music intervention, administered during regularly scheduled mathematics instruction, or continued their regular mathematics instruction with their classroom teacher. The experimental group outperformed the comparison group on music notation knowledge and the mathematical fraction completion test (i.e., not previously introduced and improper fractions). No significant group differences were found on the mathematical fraction concept test.

The four studies reviewed yielded mixed results. One RCT [21] reported positive results. The remaining three studies were (longitudinal) quasi-experimental without randomization [41] [43] and longitudinal developmental [35]. Of these three studies, one found a positive association [35], one found partial positive results [41], and one found no relation [43]. Only one of these three studies included an active control group [41]. The duration and sample sizes of two out of these three studies can be considered as strengths [43] [35]. Although possible causal relations could be tested more easily, caution is needed in interpreting findings of Bergman Nutley et al. ( 2014 ) [35], due to attrition and possible practice effects.

Other, non-musical, related skills

Two studies were identified that examined the effects of music on other, specific skills. Slater et al. ( 2015 ) [46] conducted a controlled, longitudinal study to investigate the effect of music training on speech in noise perception in 38 eight year-old children, randomly assigned to the music training program ( n = 19) or the wait-list control group ( n = 19). The authors reported a significant improvement of hearing in noise after 2 years of music training (Slater et al., 2015 ). Another longitudinal study of François et al. ( 2013 ) [45] tracked 24 eight year-old children after they were pseudo-randomly assigned to either a music training program or a painting program. They found that performance on both behavioral and electrophysiological measures of speech segmentation (i.e., the ability to extract meaningless words from a continuous flow of non-sense syllables) steadily increased across the testing sessions for the music group compared to the painting group.

Both studies, reporting positive results, employed a 2-year, longitudinal design and used valid (computer) measures to evaluate the performance of participants. Randomization, thereby reducing the risk of sampling bias, was used only in one study [46]. Information about the blinding of outcome assessors was not reported and only one out of the two studies employed an active control group [45]. Although sample sizes can be considered small, thereby limiting the external validity of findings, both studies propose an interesting direction for further research.

This review analyzed the evidence of 46 studies, dealing with five developmental domains, including the motor, social, cognitive, language, and academic domain.

With regard to the motor domain, the two studies identified suggested a positive influence of music interventions on specific motor skills (eye-hand motor sequences, discrete and continuous movements) [1] [2]. Due to the quasi-experimental design of the studies, the limited sample of participants and the inclusion of an active control group in one sub-experiment of one study only [1], clear conclusions cannot be drawn.

It cannot be concluded whether music interventions can positively influence social and emotional development as results of the nine studies reviewed [3] [4] [5] [6] [7] [8] [9] [42] are inconclusive. The findings of two experimental studies [3] [6] suggest a beneficial impact of music interventions on empathy and spontaneous cooperative and helpful behavior. The merging of the active and passive control group into one control group and the small sample size in one of the two studies, should, however, be taken into consideration. Positive findings of another study [7] turned out to be related to the level of cognitive functioning of participants in the music group. Caution is needed in drawing conclusions from the partially positive findings of Ritblatt et al. ( 2013 ) [4] and Schellenberg et al. ( 2015 ) [5] due to the design used [5] and the representativeness of the sample [4]. Two other experimental studies [8] [42] found modest effects and no effects, respectively, on social skills and self-esteem.

Regarding the language domain, 15 studies evaluating the impact of music interventions on phonological awareness and auditory processing and reading skills [12] [15] [16] [17] [18] [19] [20] [22] [23] [35], and no clear conclusions can be drawn in this area. The results of two randomized controlled trials are inconclusive. The results of four experimental studies with (stratified) randomization [13] [11] [20] [23] suggested beneficial effects of music interventions on reading skills [20] [23], however, not on phonological skills [11] [13]. Of these four studies, two included both an active and a passive control group [13] [23], allowing for a more comprehensive comparison. Of the remaining six studies, quasi-experimental (longitudinal) without randomization [10] [14] [16] [18] [22] and developmental longitudinal [35] in nature, three point to the beneficial effects of music [16] [18] [22]. The other three studies, including an active [10] or both an active and passive control group [14] or comparing participants to themselves [35], found no impact of music interventions.

With regard to the cognitive domain, seven studies reviewed provided insufficient information whether music can have a positive effect on intelligence. The results of an RCT [21] showed no effects and two experimental studies, only one of them including an active control group, yielded mixed results [30] [42]. Evidence of three quasi-experimental (longitudinal) studies [24] [25] [26] and longitudinal developmental study suggest a (partially) positive influence of music. However, an active control group was included in just one of these three studies [25]. Evidence of five (quasi-) experimental longitudinal studies seem to suggest potential benefits of music on memory. However, due to lowering generalizability of findings on one study [32], by including participants with Williams syndrome only, and methodological limitations of the other four studies (i.e., no randomization [1] [30] [31] [33] and/or no inclusion of an active control group [30] [31]), clear conclusions cannot be made. Among the six quasi-experimental studies exploring the potential influence of music on attention and EF skills, only two studies [25] [38] reported positive results. An additional five studies on working memory also seemed to suggest a positive influence. Whether or not an active control was included, the lack of randomization and the fact that working memory, attention and EF skills are difficult concepts to define, may have influenced the results obtained.

Regarding academic performance, research suggests some possible beneficial effects of music, although precise conclusions cannot be reached on the basis of reviewed studies. It cannot be concluded whether participation in a music program had a positive effect on promoting a positive approach due to the lack of randomization, the representativeness of the sample, the potential influence of parental and teacher expectations and the fact that the findings are based one study only [4]. The studies evaluating the impact of music interventions on first and second language development showed mixed findings. Regarding first language development, an RCT showed a positive effect, whereas a longitudinal study of Yang et al. ( 2014 ) reported no effects. Another two quasi-experimental studies showed improvement in second language performance [43] [44]. However, both studies included a passive control group. Of the four studies exploring the influence of music interventions on mathematics, one RCT reported positive effects [21]. Caution is needed in making causal inferences on the three remaining studies [41] [43] [35] due to the design used, the absence of randomization [41] [43] and, with regard to the study of Bergman Nutley et al. ( 2014 ) [35], attrition rate and possible practice effects. Evidence from the studies regarding the effectiveness of music on language and mathematics are reviewed separately. One can question whether there is a legitimate distinction between the two domains, as research suggest partial overlap between neural regions associated with language and arithmetic (Baldo and Dronkers, 2007 ; Cummine et al., 2014 ).

Five studies used a correlational design [9] [12] [15] [19] [37], reporting (partially) positive correlations between duration of music intervention and performance on reading and phonological awareness tasks, attention behavior, and self-esteem. Although correlational studies can provide an indication of a possible association between musical training and functioning domains, they do not allow for causal inferences.

The tool used for the methodological quality assessment allowed scoring between zero and five only. This makes a cut-off point difficult to determine. Although all domains included studies with lower quality scoring (two or less), these were more frequently found in the social and cognitive domain. This, however, does not mean that the results of these studies are invalid, but rather gives a direct for reading and interpreting them. The lower score of the study could often be explained by either unbalanced baseline characteristics, absence of randomization or missing information about blinding of outcome assessors or attrition rate. When analyzing the outcomes of the quality screening, one should take into consideration that it can be assessed with a broad range of tools. Upon applying the chosen tool, it was found that some of the items were difficult to relate to the studies at hand, but were more suitable for classical medical trials. Several criteria (including concealment of allocation and intention to treat) were negatively assessed in almost all studies, as they were not designed for the specifics of educational studies, where often it is impossible to ensure the rigid methodological quality: i.e., create double blind randomized trials.

When assessing the quality of the studied, there are several considerations regarding study design, music interventions, and the role of the teacher. In reporting on the participants, we found that little is mentioned about the intrinsic motivation of participants in the context of the intervention. As intrinsic motivation is associated with initiation and persistence of activities, level of effort and improved performance (Patall et al., 2008 ), gaining insight into the motivation of participants is important to be able to determine its impact on outcome measures. In some studies, interventions were partially provided by the authors themselves [1] [3] [6] [10] [22] [28] [36], or by parents and teachers [4]. As their expectations can have an effect on the performance of participants (the “Rosenthal effect”), one needs to be aware that observer bias rather than the intervention could cause the observed changes. Results of several studies might also have been affected by the “Hawthorne effect” i.e., a tendency of participants to alter their behavior because they are aware that they are studied. This effect cannot be ruled out or confirmed for diverse studies after screening.

Most of the study designs consisted of quasi-experimental and longitudinal designs and three studies were a RCTs. An RCT is considered as providing the strongest evidence of determining whether a cause-effect relationship exists between an intervention and outcomes (Sibbald and Roland, 1998 ) as assessment bias and confounding are minimalized. However, some research questions and settings don't permit random assignment of participants and questions may arise about the sample being representative enough of the population and the generalizability of findings to the field. As it is important to consider evidence from other methodologies as well to better understand the potential of music interventions in practice, only an RCT allows the observed effects to be causally attributed to differences between the intervention and the control group(s).

In reviewed studies, active and/or passive control groups were included in evaluating the effectiveness of a music intervention. Although showing whether participants benefit from an intervention compared to participants not receiving the intervention, passive control groups do not allow to test for intervention specific effects (Strobach and Karbach, 2016 ). Inclusion of an active control group, engaging participants in some training and activities during the intervention, can provide evidence as to whether an intervention is relatively more efficient than participating in another program (Karlsson and Bergmark, 2015 ), provided that the intervention and control group are matched on possible influencing factors and perform the same tasks.

Regarding the music interventions, studies were not uniform in their conceptualization of these music interventions. Some were very broadly defined and included listening, singing, instrumental playing, performing, movement, and musical creativity. While others, especially focused on the acquisition of non-musical skills, were more precisely defined and designed. Differences in musical content deserve attention in likely contributing to the outcomes of music interventions. Interventions in groups may have additional benefits of social interaction and motivation above the intervention itself compared to individual interventions which could have played a role in its final outcomes. In this review, the role of the teacher also emerged as a significant issue. 18 of the included studies employed (professional) music teachers and 16 reported at least partly positive outcomes. Teaching music requires many competencies. Strong teaching skills without musical skills and knowledge is not sufficient and vice versa. Research points not only to musical content knowledge, but also to pedagogical content knowledge and non-pedagogical professional knowledge (Ballantyne and Packer, 2004 ). By the way they teach, they play an important role in the teacher-child relationship which may have in turn implications for children's behavioral and academic adjustment (Furrer and Skinner, 2003 ). Therefore, teachers may also be an important factor in the context in which the effectiveness of music interventions is researched.

Another point of attention when describing the effects of music interventions on the development of children is the methodological accuracy and variety of different approaches the researchers took in their studies. Being the most powerful research design for evaluating interventions, further RCTs are needed to determine whether music interventions are effective in stimulating development in children. However, particularly in the domain of music interventions in schools, some requirements such as blinding, randomization, and controlling for potential sources of variability are often difficult if not impossible to achieve and RCTs may create an artificial situation in which findings may not always be applied to everyday practice. While we acknowledge the need for high-quality research methodology, it is important to find a balance between the externally imposed methodological standards and the drive to investigate a said phenomenon in its natural environment. As qualitative research can provide more insight into the characteristics of the intervention and can generate potential hypotheses for quantitative research, combining qualitative and quantitative research can give more comprehensive and integrated insights in potential effects of music interventions.

In conclusion, although the underlying mechanisms are not always clear, evidence of reviewed studies seems suggestive of some beneficial effects. Having a clearer view of effects and possible influencing factors may pave the way for further research on the influence of music on the developing child.

Author contributions

ED the main author and executor of the research and participated in data collection and analysis as well as article writing process. ES was a second reviewer of the selected articles, contributed to development of methodology, data extraction, and analyses as well as final comments on the article writing. FF and SvH contributed to the idea of the development of the article as well as development of the research methodology and provided feedback during the article writing stage.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

  • Baldo J. V., Dronkers N. F. (2007). Neural correlates of arithmetic and language comprehension: a common substrate? Neuropsychologia 45 , 229–235. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.07.014 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ballantyne J., Packer J. (2004). Effectiveness of preservice music teacher education programs: perceptions of early-career music teachers . Music Educ. Res. 6 , 299–312. 10.1080/1461380042000281749 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bergman Nutley S., Darki F., Klingberg T. (2014). Music practice is associated with development of working memory during childhood and adolescence . Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7 :926. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00926 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Besson M., Chobert J., Marie C. (2011). Transfer of training between music and speech: common processing, attention, and memory . Front. Psychol. 2 :94. 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00094 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bhide A., Power A., Goswami U. (2013). A rhythmic musical intervention for poor readers: a comparison of efficacy with a letter-based intervention . Mind Brain Educ. 7 , 113–123. 10.1111/mbe.12016 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bonacina S., Cancer A., Lanzi P. L., Lorusso M. L., Antonietti A. (2015). Improving reading skills in students with dyslexia: the efficacy of a sublexical training with rhythmic background . Front. Psychol. 6 :1510. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01510 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brodsky W., Sulkin I. (2011). Handclapping songs: a spontaneous platform for child development among 5–10-year-old children . Early Child Dev. Care 181 , 1111–1136. 10.1080/03004430.2010.517837 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bugos J., Jacobs E. (2012). Composition instruction and cognitive performance: results of a pilot study . Res. Issues Music Educ. 10 :2 Available online at: http://ir.stthomas.edu/rime/vol10/iss1/2 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cogo-Moreira H., Andriolo R. B., Yazigi L., Ploubidis G. B., Brandão de Ávila C. R., Mari J. J. (2012). Music education for improving reading skills in children and adolescents with dyslexia . Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 15 :CD009133 10.1002/14651858.CD009133.pub2 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cogo-Moreira H., Brandão de Ávila C. R., Ploubidis G. B., Mari J. D. J. (2013). Effectiveness of music education for the improvement of reading skills and academic achievement in young poor readers: a pragmatic cluster-randomized, controlled clinical trial . PLoS ONE 8 :e59984. 10.1371/journal.pone.0059984 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Corrigall K. A., Trainor L. J. (2011). Associations between length of music training and reading skills in children . Music Percept. 29 , 147–155. 10.1525/mp.2011.29.2.147 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Costa-Giomi E. (2004). Effects of three years of piano instruction on children's academic achievement, school performance and self-esteem . Psychol. Music 32 , 139–152. 10.1177/0305735604041491 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Courey S. J., Balogh E., Siker J. R., Paik J. (2012). Academic music: music instruction to engage third-grade students in learning basic fraction concepts . Educ. Stud. Math. 81 , 251–278. 10.1007/s10649-012-9395-9 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cummine J., Szepesvari E., Chouinard B., Hanif W., Georgiou G. K. (2014). A functional investigation of RAN letters, digits, and objects: how similar are they? Behav. Brain Res. 275 , 157–165. 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.08.038 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Degé F., Schwarzer G. (2011). The effect of a music program on phonological awareness in preschoolers . Front. Psychol. 2 :124. 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00124 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Degé F., Wehrum S., Stark R., Schwarzer G. (2011). The influence of two years of school music training in secondary school on visual and auditory memory . Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 8 , 608–623. 10.1080/17405629.2011.590668 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Degé F., Wehrum S., Stark R., Schwarzer G. (2014). Music lessons and academic self-concept in 12-to 14-year-old children . Music Sci. 18 , 203–215. 10.1177/1029864914523283 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Escalda J., Lemos S. M. A., França C. C. (2011). Auditory processing and phonological awareness skills of five-year-old children with and without musical experience . J. Soc. Bras. Fonoaudiol. 23 , 258–263. 10.1590/S2179-64912011000300012 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Flaugnacco E., Lopez L., Terribili C., Montico M., Zoia S., Schön D. (2015). Music training increases phonological awareness and reading skills in developmental dyslexia: a randomized control trial . PLoS ONE 10 :e0138715. 10.1371/journal.pone.0138715 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fonseca-Mora M. C., Jara-Jiménez P., Gómez-Domínguez M. (2015). Musical plus phonological input for young foreign language readers . Front. Psychol. 6 :286. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00286 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Forgeard M., Winner E., Norton A., Schlaug G. (2008). Practicing a musical instrument in childhood is associated with enhanced verbal ability and nonverbal reasoning . PLoS ONE 3 :e3566. 10.1371/journal.pone.0003566 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • François C., Chobert J., Besson M., Schön D. (2013). Music training for the development of speech segmentation . Cereb. Cortex 23 , 2038–2043. 10.1093/cercor/bhs180 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Furrer C., Skinner E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a factor in children's academic engagement and performance . J. Educ. Psychol. 95 , 148–162. 10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.148 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gioia G. A., Isquith P. K., Guy S. C., Kenworthy L. (2000). Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function: Professional Manual . Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grant M. J., Booth A. (2009). A typology of reviews: an analysis of 14 review types and associated methodologies . Health Inform. Librar. J. 26 , 91–108. 10.1111/j.1471-1842.2009.00848.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Habib M., Lardy C., Desiles T., Commeiras C., Chobert J., Besson M. (2016). Music and Dyslexia: a new musical training method to improve reading and related disorders . Front. Psychol. 7 :26. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00026 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Herrera L., Lorenzo O., Defior S., Fernandez-Smith G., Costa-Giomi E. (2011). Effects of phonological and musical training on the reading readiness of native-and foreign-Spanish-speaking children . Psychol. Music 39 , 68–81. 10.1177/0305735610361995 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ho Y. C., Cheung M. C., Chan A. S. (2003). Music training improves verbal but not visual memory: cross-sectional and longitudinal explorations in children . Neuropsychology 17 , 439–450. 10.1037/0894-4105.17.3.439 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Janus M., Lee Y., Moreno S., Bialystok E. (2016). Effects of short-term music and second-language training on executive control . J. Exp. Child Psychol. 144 , 84–97. 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.009 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Janzen T. B., Thompson W. F., Ranvaud R. (2014). A developmental study of the effect of music training on timed movements . Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8 :801 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00801 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jaschke A. C., Eggermont L. H., Honing H., Scherder E. J. (2013). Music education and its effect on intellectual abilities in children: a systematic review . Rev. Neurosci. 24 , 665–675. 10.1515/revneuro-2013-0023 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jentschke S., Koelsch S. (2009). Musical training modulates the development of syntax processing in children . Neuroimage 47 , 735–744. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.090 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Karlsson P., Bergmark A. (2015). Compared with what? An analysis of control-group types in Cochrane and Campbell reviews of psychosocial treatment efficacy with substance use disorders . Addiction 110 , 420–428. 10.1111/add.12799 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kaviani H., Mirbaha H., Pournaseh M., Sagan O. (2014). Can music lessons increase the performance of preschool children in IQ tests? Cogn. Process. 15 , 77–84. 10.1007/s10339-013-0574-0 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Khalil A. K., Minces V., McLoughlin G., Chiba A. (2013). Group rhythmic synchrony and attention in children . Front. Psychol. 4 :564. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00564 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kirschner S., Tomasello M. (2010). Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children . Evol. Hum. Behav. 31 , 354–364. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04.004 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maloy M., Peterson R. (2014). A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of music interventions for children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder . Music Mind Brain 24 , 328–339. 10.1037/pmu0000083 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Martens M. A., Jungers M. K., Steele A. L. (2011). Effect of musical experience on verbal memory in Williams syndrome: evidence from a novel word learning task . Neuropsychologia 49 , 3093–3102. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.016 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mehr S. A., Schachner A., Katz R. C., Spelke E. S. (2013). Two randomized trials provide no consistent evidence for nonmusical cognitive benefits of brief preschool music enrichment . PLoS ONE 8 :e82007. 10.1371/journal.pone.0082007 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Melnyk B. M., Fineout-Overholt E. (2005). Making the case for evidence-based practice , in Evidence-Based Practice in Nursing and Healthcare: A Guide to Best Practice , eds Melnyk B. M., Fineout-Overholt E. (New York, NY: Lippincott; Williams & Wilkins; ), 3–24. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Miendlarzewska E. A., Trost W. J. (2014). How musical training affects cognitive development: rhythm, reward and other modulating variables . Front. Neurosci. 7 :279. 10.3389/fnins.2013.00279 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moreno S., Bialystok E., Barac R., Schellenberg E. G., Cepeda N. J., Chau T. (2011a). Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function . Psychol. Sci. 22 , 1425–1433. 10.1177/0956797611416999 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moreno S., Friesen D., Bialystok E. (2011b). Effect of music training on promoting preliteracy skills: preliminary causal evidence . Music Percept. 29 , 165–172. 10.1525/mp.2011.29.2.165 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moritz C., Yampolsky S., Papadelis G., Thomson J., Wolf M. (2013). Links between early rhythm skills, musical training, and phonological awareness . Read. Writ. 26 , 739–769. 10.1007/s11145-012-9389-0 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Patall E. A., Cooper H., Robinson J. C. (2008). The effects of choice on intrinsic motivation and related outcomes: a meta-analysis of research findings . Psychol. Bull. 134 , 270–300. 10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.270 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pelham W. E., Jr., Waschbusch D. A., Hoza B., Gnagy E. M., Greiner A. R., Sams S. E., et al.. (2011). Music and video as distractors for boys with ADHD in the classroom: comparison with controls, individual differences, and medication effects . J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 39 , 1085–1098. 10.1007/s10802-011-9529-z [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Portowitz A., Peppler K. A., Downton M. (2014). In Harmony: a technology-based music education model to enhance musical understanding and general learning skills . Int. J. Music Educ . 32 , 242–260. 10.1177/0255761413517056 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rabinowitch T. C., Cross I., Burnard P. (2013). Long-term musical group interaction has a positive influence on empathy in children . Psychol. Music 41 , 484–498. 10.1177/0305735612440609 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rautenberg I. (2013). The effects of musical training on the decoding skills of German-speaking primary school children . J. Res. Read. 38 , 1–17. 10.1111/jrir.12010 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rickard N. S., Appelman P., James R., Murphy F., Gill A., Bambrick C. (2013). Orchestrating life skills: the effect of increased school-based music classes on children's social competence and self-esteem . Int. J. Music Educ. 31 , 292–309. 10.1177/0255761411434824 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rickard N. S., Bambrick C. J., Gill A. (2012). Absence of widespread psychosocial and cognitive effects of school-based music instruction in 10–13-year-old students . Int. J. Music Educ. 30 , 57–78. 10.1177/0255761411431399 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rickard N. S., Vasquez J. T., Murphy F., Gill A., Toukhsati S. R. (2010). Benefits of a classroom based instrumental music program on verbal memory of primary school children: a longitudinal study . Austral. J. Music Educ. 1 , 36–47. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ritblatt S., Longstreth S., Hokoda A., Cannon B. N., Weston J. (2013). Can music enhance school-readiness socioemotional skills? J. Res. Childhood Educ. 27 , 257–266. 10.1080/02568543.2013.796333 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Roden I., Grube D., Bongard S., Kreutz G. (2014). Does music training enhance working memory performance ? Findings from a quasi-experimental longitudinal study . Psychol. Music 42 , 284–298. 10.1177/0305735612471239 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Roden I., Kreutz G., Bongard S. (2012). Effects of a school-based instrumental music program on verbal and visual memory in primary school children: a longitudinal study . Front. Psychol. 3 :572. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00572 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Särkämö T., Ripollés P., Vepsäläinen H., Autti T., Silvennoinen H. M., Salli E., et al.. (2014). Structural changes induced by daily music listening in the recovering brain after middle cerebral artery stroke: a voxel-based morphometry study . Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8 :245. 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00245 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schellenberg E. G. (2004). Music lessons enhance IQ . Psychol. Sci. 15 , 511–514. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00711.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schellenberg E. G. (2011). Examining the association between music lessons and intelligence . Br. J. Psychol. 102 , 283–302. 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02000.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schellenberg E. G., Mankarious M. (2012). Music training and emotion comprehension in childhood . Emotion 12 , 887–891. 10.1037/a0027971 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schellenberg E. G., Corrigall K. A., Dys S. P., Malti T. (2015). Group music training and children's prosocial skills . PLoS ONE 10 :e0141449. 10.1371/journal.pone.0141449 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sibbald B., Roland M. (1998). Understanding controlled trials: why are randomised controlled trials important? Br. Med. J. 316 :201. 10.1136/bmj.316.7126.201 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Slater J., Skoe E., Strait D. L., O'Connell S., Thompson E., Kraus N. (2015). Music training improves speech-in-noise perception: longitudinal evidence from a community-based music program . Behav. Brain Res. 291 , 244–252. 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.05.026 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Slater J., Strait D. L., Skoe E., O'Connell S., Thompson E., Kraus N. (2014). Longitudinal effects of group music instruction on literacy skills in low-income children . PLoS ONE 9 :e113383. 10.1371/journal.pone.0113383 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Southgate D. E., Roscigno V. J. (2009). The impact of music on childhood and adolescent achievement . Soc. Sci. Q. 90 , 4–21. 10.1111/j.1540-6237.2009.00598.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Standley J. M. (2008). Does music instruction help children learn to read? Evidence of a meta-analysis . Update 27 , 17–32. 10.1177/8755123308322270 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Strait D. L., Kraus N., Parbery-Clark A., Ashley R. (2010). Musical experience shapes top-down auditory mechanisms: evidence from masking and auditory attention performance . Hear. Res. 261 , 22–29. 10.1016/j.heares.2009.12.021 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Strobach T., Karbach J. (2016). Cognitive Training: An Overview of Features and Applications . Berlin: Springer-Verlag. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Suthers L., Niland A. (2007). An exploration of young children's engagement with music experiences , in Listen to their Voices: Research and Practice in Earlychildhood Music , eds Smithrim K., Upitis R. (Waterloo, ON: Canadian Music Educators' Association; ), 19–32. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Swaminathan S., Gopinath J. K. (2013). Music training and second-language English comprehension and vocabulary skills in Indian children . Psychol. Stud. 58 , 164–170. 10.1007/s12646-013-0180-3 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yang H., Ma W., Gong D., Hu J., Yao D. (2014). A Longitudinal study on children's music training experience and academic development . Sci. Rep. 4 :5854. 10.1038/srep05854 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yazejian N., Peisner-Feinberg E. S. (2009). Effects of a preschool music and movement curriculum on children's language skills . NHSA Dialog 12 , 327–341. 10.1080/15240750903075255 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zuk J., Benjamin C., Kenyon A., Gaab N. (2014). Behavioral and neural correlates of executive functioning in musicians and non-musicians . PLoS ONE 9 :e99868. 10.1371/journal.pone.0099868 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

Generative Artificial Intelligence in education: Think piece by Stefania Giannini

generative ai in education

Artificial Intelligence tools open new horizons for education, but we urgently need to take action to ensure we integrate them into learning systems on our terms. That is the core message of UNESCO’s new paper on generative AI and the future of education . In her think piece, UNESCO Assistant Director-General for Education, Stefania Giannini expresses her concerns that the checks and balances applied to teaching materials are not being used to the implementation of generative AI. While highlighting that AI tools create new prospects for learning, she underscores that regulations can only be built once the proper research has been conducted.

Readiness of schools to regulate the use of AI tools in education

In May, a UNESCO global survey of over 450 schools and universities found that fewer than 10% have developed institutional policies and/or formal guidance concerning the use of generative AI applications. The paper observes that in most countries, the time, steps and authorizations needed to validate a new textbook far surpass those required to move generative AI utilities into schools and classrooms. Textbooks are usually evaluated for accuracy of content, age-appropriateness, relevance of teaching and accuracy of content, cultural and social suitability which encompasses checks to protect against bias, before being used in the classroom.

Education systems must set own rules

The education sector cannot rely on the corporate creators of AI to regulate its own work. To vet and validate new and complex AI applications for formal use in school, UNESCO recommends that ministries of education build their capacities in coordination with other regulatory branches of government, in particular those regulating technologies.

Potential to undermine the status of teachers and the necessity of schools

The paper underscores that education should remain a deeply human act rooted in social interaction. It recalls that during the COVID-19 pandemic, when digital technology became the primary medium for education, students suffered both academically and socially. The paper warns us that generative AI in particular has the potential to both undermine the authority and status of teachers, and to strengthen calls for further automation of education: Teacher-less schools, and school-less education. It emphasizes that well-run schools, coupled with sufficient teacher numbers, training and salaries must be prioritized.

Education spending must focus on fundamental learning objectives

The paper argues that investment in schools and teachers, is the only way to solve the problem that today, at the dawn of the AI Era, 244 million children and youth are out of school and more than 770 million people are non-literate. Evidence shows that good schools and teachers can resolve this persistent educational challenge – yet the world continues to underfund them.

UNESCO’s response to generative AI in education

UNESCO is steering the global dialogue with policy-makers, EdTech partners, academia and civil society. The first global meeting of Ministers of Education took place in May 2023 and the Organization is developing policy guidelines on the use of generative AI in education and research, as well as frameworks of AI competencies for students and teachers for school education. These will be launched during the Digital Learning Week , which will take place at UNESCO Headquarters in Paris on 4-7 September 2023. The UNESCO Global Education Monitoring Report 2023 to be published on 26 July 2023 will focus on the use of technology in education.

UNESCO’s Recommendation on the Ethics of Artificial Intelligence

UNESCO produced the first-ever global standard on AI ethics – the ‘Recommendation on the Ethics of Artificial Intelligence’ in November 2021. This framework was adopted by all 193 Member States. The Recommendation stresses that governments must ensure that AI always adheres to the principles of safety, inclusion, diversity, transparency and quality.

0000385877

Related items

  • Artificial intelligence
  • Future of education
  • Educational technology
  • Digital learning week
  • Educational trends
  • Topics: Display
  • See more add

More on this subject

Global Network of Learning Cities webinar ‘Countering climate disinformation: strengthening global citizenship education and media literacy’

Other recent idea

Reimagining education financing through international cooperation

Winners of national penmanship contest crowned as handwriting is 'having a moment'

Namuun Baasanbol poses for a photo with her handwriting.

It’s regarded, hands down, as the Super Bowl of penmanship tournaments.

The Zaner-Bloser National Handwriting Contest, now in its 33rd year, crowned its 2024 grand champions on Monday, rewarding nine students from six states for their picture-perfect letters.

Ten-year old Zita Miller of White Bear Lake, Minnesota, took top honors in the fifth grade category. Her winning submission was one of the contest’s 80,000 entries.

“I like handwriting because it’s like art, drawing swirls and vines and curls,” Miller said, adding that she enjoys penning original mystery stories by hand.

Zita Miller poses for a photograph.

Namuun Baasanbold, from Carmel, Indiana, was named grand champion in the first grade category, and said she likes to give handwritten “love notes” to family and friends.

“Writing by hand makes me feel special,” she said.

The contest celebrates a centuries-old practice, but the victories come as handwriting is experiencing a kind of renaissance in the U.S. In January, California became the 22nd state to require cursive to be taught in schools — a significant jump from 2016, when just 12 states mandated it.

At the same time, various studies published over the past decade have detailed how writing with pencil and paper can benefit memory, cognitive development, reading comprehension and fine motor skills.

“Handwriting is definitely having a moment,” said Sharon Quirk-Silva, a member of the California state Assembly who sponsored the bill. She said she heard from people from all over the country who penned “beautifully handwritten notes” of support for the new law.

“We live in a very polarized nation. So many issues are contentious. But with this handwriting bill, we had full bipartisan support and goodwill. The importance of handwriting is something people seem to agree on,” she said.

Quirk-Silva said she backed the bill, in large part, because of her own experience — before becoming a lawmaker, she taught elementary school for 25 years.

“For years, technology has been taking over the curriculum in schools, with many kids being dormant in front of the screen, using two or three screens a day. Now, there’s a feeling of, ‘Let’s get pens and pencils back in kids’ hands,’” she said.

Although the California law mandates that first through sixth graders in the state receive cursive instruction, Quirk-Silva said she believes that writing by hand — in print or cursive — is an important language arts tool.

“It’s a way of slowing down a little bit, taking your thoughts from your brain to your hand and physically doing the writing,” she said.

Sophia Vinci-Booher, an assistant professor of psychology and human development at Vanderbilt University, said her research found that writing by hand enabled preschool students to form connections in the brain that likely support early letter recognition.

For that study , published in 2016, 20 children were asked to practice certain letters by writing them over and over, and practice others by pressing a button.

“Then we asked the children to go into an MRI scanner and look at those letters they’d been practicing,” Vinci-Booher explained. Her team analyzed the children’s brain activity to assess the functional connectivity between different areas of their brains.

“We found that the connection was stronger with letters they wrote by hand than those they tapped,” she said.

The research underscores the importance of the physical act of forming symbols, Vinci-Booher added.

“Writing by hand is a good thing for kids because it supports early reading development and it engages the fine motor system, which is developmentally important,” she said.

A 2021 study measured people’s brain activity during a memory task, this time finding that University of Tokyo students exhibited stronger activity and better recall after they had written information down on paper than when they did on a smartphone or even with a stylus on a tablet. The researchers suggested that the physical act of writing on paper provides the brain more details that trigger memory, and concluded that using paper notebooks can help students retain information in part because of their “tangible permanence.”

A similar study published in January compared the brain activity of students at the Norwegian University of Science and Technology who took notes by hand to the activity of those who typed their notes. The findings suggested that the students who wrote by hand had higher levels of electrical activity across a wide range of brain regions responsible for sensory processing and memory.

The results come as little surprise to many educators.

“I’ve seen firsthand that the kids learn more when they write by hand,” said Geeta Kadakia, who teaches second through fifth grade at the DAV Montessori School in Houston. “The lightbulb goes off through those achievements in handwriting, and handwriting leads to achievements in other areas, even math. When students make their numbers more neatly, their math scores improve.”

Laura Gajderowicz taught elementary school for 33 years in Indiana before retiring in 2022. She said she worried as she watched handwriting take a back seat to technology in U.S. classrooms in the early 2000s.

“Writing by hand does so much to help with the development of a student’s eye-hand coordination,” Gajderowicz said, adding: “I’m not against technology — I just think there’s a place at the table for both technology and handwriting when it comes to learning.”

This year, Gajderowicz served as a regional judge in the Zaner-Bloser contest.

“I was pleasantly surprised to see how many entries we had, especially from children in the upper grades,” she said.

Gajderowicz selected winners using criteria that analyzed the mechanics and precision of the letters students wrote, including their shapes, sizes, slant and spacing.

Contestants were asked to write the sentence, “The quick brown fox jumps over the lazy dog,” because it includes the entire alphabet, as well as a sentence explaining why handwriting makes them a better reader and writer.

Namuun Baasanbold’s entry.

Baasanbold said she was “over the moon” to find out she won: “I screamed and celebrated with friends at a restaurant with pizza and an appetizer and a sundae for dessert,” she said.

Her prizes include a trophy and $500 — plus bragging rights.

“I like to use my handwriting to impress people,” she said.

Mary Pflum is a national field producer for NBC News, based in New York.

Doctors can do more to help prevent gun violence, USF paper says

  • Sam Ogozalek Times staff

Doctors can do more to help prevent gun violence and offer counseling on firearms safety, according to a review by University of South Florida researchers, including a medical student who survived the 2018 mass shooting at Marjory Stoneman Douglas High School in Parkland.

The review, published in February in the journal Advances in Pediatrics, noted that many doctors believe they should talk to patients about firearms, but often don’t because of time constraints, a lack of training and discomfort with the topic.

In Florida, a law passed in 2011 limited what questions doctors could ask patients about guns. But a federal appeals court found the restrictions to be unconstitutional and struck them down six years later.

The law “had a very chilling effect I think for a lot of providers,” said Cameron Nereim, one of the review’s authors and an assistant professor of pediatrics at USF who focuses on patients ages 12 to 25 in Tampa.

The review noted that the American Medical Association calls gun violence a public health crisis and firearm-related injuries were the leading cause of death for U.S. children in 2020, surpassing motor vehicle crashes.

Doctors can ask patients about whether guns are kept at home and if they are locked, unloaded and separated from ammunition, according to the review.

USF medical student Nikhita Nookala, who survived the shooting at Marjory Stoneman Douglas High School and covered it as a reporter at The Eagle Eye, the school’s student newspaper, contributed to the review.

The Tampa Bay Times spoke with Nereim and Nookala about their work, which was published with two other authors from North Carolina and Texas medical centers.

The interviews have been edited for clarity and length.

What are the major takeaways you hope physicians get from this paper?

Nookala: It’s important to assess patients’ risks and to see if you can do some harm reduction in terms of encouraging safe storage or just telling kids to be careful around guns.

It doesn’t have to be super political. It’s just about children’s safety at the end of the day.

The paper noted some of the major barriers to physicians counseling on firearms safety are the lack of formal training, lack of confidence that patients will follow their recommendations and low perceived self-efficacy. Are there any ways to address that?

Nereim: I think there are. ... One of the things is how do we do a better job of incorporating this into our training, whether that’s while you’re going through your residency program, while you’re going through your medical school, making sure these things are actually being plugged into our curricula.

Keep up with Tampa Bay’s top headlines

Subscribe to our free DayStarter newsletter

You’re all signed up!

Want more of our free, weekly newsletters in your inbox? Let’s get started.

Is there a particular stage of education where you think this would be best emphasized?

Nereim: I think medical school, before students have even differentiated in terms of what specialty or subspecialty they’ll be choosing, that’s a great opportunity to make sure we’re relaying this information.

In school so far, have people talked about gun violence as a public health issue?

Nookala: Everyone acknowledges that it’s become a problem and it’s getting worse. I think a lot of people feel very helpless because it’s something that is hard to bring up.

You’re not accusing someone of “You don’t store your gun safely, you’re putting your child in danger.” ... You want to (tread) the line of “Oh, you have guns in the home, that’s fine, are you storing them safely, are (you) using gun locks?”

It’s just small things that you can remind people, nicely, without discouraging them from coming back.

During routine visits, do you ask patients or patients’ parents whether there are guns in the home, if they are locked, securely unloaded (and) separated from ammunition?

Nereim: Yes, we do.

With the ongoing mental health crisis that’s involving young people ... I think it becomes even more critical that we do our due diligence and we ask those questions about things like firearm ownership ... how ammunition is being stored, what level of training the family members or the kids themselves have in terms of actually using these objects.

How do those conversations normally go for you? Are they difficult?

Nereim: When you’re able to successfully create that nonjudgemental space ... the vast majority of patients and families are really receptive to the questions we’re asking.

Do you ask patients more often than not? Or do you ask their parents?

Nereim: I tend to do both.

Do you ask firearm-related questions in every visit with a patient, or do you only ask them with someone who would be considered high risk?

Nereim: I wouldn’t say we ask it at every visit. I think we’re getting better at making this more a universal thing that we’re screening for, in the same way that 15, 20 years ago we would make sure at every visit we’re asking “Are you wearing your safety helmet if you’re riding your bike? Are you buckling your seat belt if you’re riding in a car?” We’re moving in that direction.

It happens a lot more consistently in these higher-risk situations, so if you have a patient who’s experiencing mood symptoms, depression or maybe even anger, irritability, impulsive behaviors.

(The paper suggested) that physicians can link those at risk of gun violence to other programs that offer support in the community. Do you do that regularly? How common is that?

Nereim: It’s extremely common. ... In the space where I work, probably anywhere from 50 to 70% of patients may have some significant social need. ... A lot of young people confide in me that they’re fearful just to be outside and to be walking in the place where they live because there have been times where they’ve heard gunshots. Or they know there was a shooting that occurred in that same square, that same block.

As you can imagine, when you have these kinds of social needs that manifest over time, there can be pretty major health consequences.

Why is this research important to you?

Nookala: In the aftermath of the (Marjory Stoneman Douglas High School) shooting, the focus was on Parkland, and Parkland became this beacon of gun violence prevention. But the reality is that outside of that one incident, which was horrible, gun violence doesn’t really occur in cities like Parkland every day. It occurs in cities like Tampa every day in marginalized communities. … The focus needs to be shifted back to that.

Training to be a doctor, I wanted to know more about ways you could make changes on an interpersonal level with your patients without being involved in these really political movements that oftentimes (are) just kind of doing nothing or (do) a little something and it gets reversed a few years later. It’s frustrating to watch that.

Nereim: Violence leads to more violence. The only really, truly effective way for us to move forward as a society is we just have to realize that prevention is incredibly important. … I just don’t think we can ignore the role this is playing in the lives of our patients and their families.

Sam Ogozalek is a reporter covering the healthcare system and mental health. He can be reached at [email protected].

MORE FOR YOU

  • Advertisement

ONLY AVAILABLE FOR SUBSCRIBERS

The Tampa Bay Times e-Newspaper is a digital replica of the printed paper seven days a week that is available to read on desktop, mobile, and our app for subscribers only. To enjoy the e-Newspaper every day, please subscribe.

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) Music Education and Effective Teaching: Perspectives from a

    research paper about music teaching

  2. ≫ Understanding Music Therapy Free Essay Sample on Samploon.com

    research paper about music teaching

  3. Exploring Research in Music Education and Music Therapy: Phillips

    research paper about music teaching

  4. Music Careers Research Paper and Poster Project by The Musical Rose

    research paper about music teaching

  5. 😂 Music education research paper. 20 Potential Topics For Your Research

    research paper about music teaching

  6. The New Handbook of Research on Music Teaching and Learning

    research paper about music teaching

VIDEO

  1. Downtime

  2. The Language of Music

  3. Music Advocacy Presentation

  4. Research Gap Identification Using Three Easy Ways|| Research Objective & Gap Finding's Tips||#Bangla

  5. Why Music?

  6. The Power of Emotional Intelligence in Music Teaching

COMMENTS

  1. Journal of Research in Music Education: Sage Journals

    SUBMIT PAPER. Journal of Research in Music Education is a quarterly, peer-reviewed journal comprising reports of original research related to music teaching and learning. The wide range of topics includes various aspects of music pedagogy, history, and philosophy, and addresses vocal, instrumental, and general music at all levels, from early ...

  2. PDF s Inspiring music teachers: A study of fo r mu sic m e

    educators who give insight into what they believe are the inspiring qualities of music teacher practice. The research comes from a larger study by the author (Robinson, 2015) and seeks to understand what is valued in music teacher practice. This paper reveals five characteristics of inspiring music teaching: knowledge with passion, the ...

  3. (PDF) The power of music: Its impact on the intellectual, social and

    The paper synthesises indicative research findings and considers the implications for . education. ! ... Journal of Research in Music Education, 46, 173-181. ! 26!! Gromko, J. (2005) The effect of ...

  4. (PDF) Music Education and Effective Teaching: Perspectives from a

    University of Padova. Abstract. Effective teaching is an essential condition for. fostering learning in instrumental and vocal music. education. The role of the teacher in music lessons. has to be ...

  5. Full article: The digital 'turn' in music education (editorial)

    View PDF View EPUB. The global COVID-19 pandemic has disrupted music education across the world, resulting in radical changes to the field of practice, accelerating a 'turn' toward online digital musical experiences. This digital 'turn' is likely to influence the future of music education in a variety of complex and inter-connected ways.

  6. Effective Music Teachers and Effective Music Teaching Today: A ...

    (1) Background: This systematic review focuses on identifying the main features of effective music teachers and teaching recently examined in the educational and psychological literature. It aims to identify how recent studies have discussed the promotion of effectiveness in the context of both preservice and in-service music teachers. (2) Methods: A search in the main scientific databases for ...

  7. Music Education Research

    Music Education Research is an international refereed journal which draws its contributions from a wide community of researchers. The focus is firmly on research, and the journal provides an international forum for cross-cultural investigations and discussions relating to all areas of music education. Music Education Research welcomes articles ...

  8. PDF How Music Education Helps Students Learn, Achieve, and Succeed Music

    Music education readies students for learning by helping to develop their basic mental skills and capacities. Music instruction impacts learning in the following ways: ... Journal of Research in Music Education, 40(3), 225-235. Strategic National Arts Alumni Project (SNAAP). (2010). Forks in the Road: The Many Paths of Arts Alumni: Strategic

  9. (PDF) An overview of research on music and learning

    Several researchers made mentioned on the benefits of music and songs. Scripp (2002) said that music could promote individual's cognitive and social-emotional development across all disciplines ...

  10. Music Interventions and Child Development: A Critical Review and

    This paper reviews the latest evidence on the effect of musical interventions on the development of primary school-aged children. Four electronic databases were searched from January 2010 through June 2016 using music, music instruction, music education, music lesson, music training, development, child, student, and pupil as key words for the ...

  11. Music Education Research: Vol 26, No 1 (Current issue)

    Music Education Research, Volume 26, Issue 1 (2024) Special Issue: Challenging approaches to music curricula and literacy today. Guest Editors: Hilary McQueen and Esther Cavett.

  12. Research on Music Teaching and Creation Based on Deep Learning

    This paper discusses the value and application strategies of deep learning in junior high school music teaching and hopes to provide some reference for all educational colleagues (Zhang and Nauman 2020). Research on Music Teaching and Creation Based on Deep Learning. Under the background of quality education, music learning is also changing ...

  13. Longitudinal Research on Music Education and Child Development

    Longitudinal research offers unparalleled insights into child development in and through music. This type of research design is well aligned with two central tenets of education: the notion that learning is an interactive process that unfolds over the course of time, and that learning promotes changes to one's knowledge, beliefs, and behaviors (Ambrose et al., 2010).

  14. Full article: Music in early childhood teacher education: raising

    This body of research would be essential for teacher educators, school leaders, and policymakers to understand how best to support ECE teachers in music education, for the benefit of young children (Campbell & Scott-Kassner, Citation 2019).

  15. PDF The impact of participation in music on learning mathematics

    Music psychologists have established that some forms of musical activity improve intellectual performance, spatial-temporal reasoning and other skills advantageous for learning. In this research, the potential of active music-making for improving pupils' achievement in spatial- temporal reasoning was investigated.

  16. On the creative teaching in primary school music classroom

    Music creative teaching has a positive influence on developing students 'consciousness of music creation, students' creative thinking and cultivating innovative practice ability. This paper ...

  17. Use of Music and Songs as Pedagogical Tools in Teaching ...

    The purpose of this research paper is to demonstrate how music can be used in the classroom by describing several methods and providing resources, as well as to demonstrate why music can benefit foreign language learning by reviewing research on the impact of music-related methods on EFL learning and teaching. The researchers relied upon the previous studies and critical and scholarly texts.

  18. PDF National Education Policy 2020 and The Significance of Music Education

    Paper Received On: 21 DEC 2021 Peer Reviewed On: 31 DEC 2021 Published On: 1 JAN 2022 ... Music education is a field of study associated with tutoring and literacy music. It touches on ... Research Universities, Teaching Universities and Autonomous degree-granting colleges. NEP 2020 aims to provide autonomy to all higher education institutions ...

  19. Generative Artificial Intelligence in education: Think piece by

    The paper warns us that generative AI in particular has the potential to both undermine the authority and status of teachers, and to strengthen calls for further automation of education: Teacher-less schools, and school-less education. ... of Education took place in May 2023 and the Organization is developing policy guidelines on the use of ...

  20. (PDF) Technology in Music Education

    Technology in Music Education. November 2023 · Canadian Journal of Learning and Technology / La revue canadienne de l'apprentissage et de la technologie. Adita Maharaj. Akini Gill. This study ...

  21. Nine students win national handwriting contest

    Nine students won this year's national handwriting contest. A growing number of states are requiring cursive instruction, and research supports the benefits of writing on paper.

  22. Doctors can do more to help prevent gun violence, USF paper says

    Published 5 minutes ago. Doctors can do more to help prevent gun violence and offer counseling on firearms safety, according to a review by University of South Florida researchers, including a ...

  23. A systematic literature review of Chinese music education studies

    Based on findings from a large meta-data-based literature survey, this article is intended to provide a comprehensive synthesis of key features of China's music education system as seen through the lens of n = 116 major research studies, drawn from a total of N = 3,257 high-impact Chinese journal articles published during 2007 to 2019.The synthesis suggests that (1) education reform ...

  24. A Review of the Literature on the Relationship of Music Education to

    emotional effects have also become a topics of research. The concept of art education, in particular education through art, was defined by Read (Kiss, 2017) who contended that the ... its social and emotional effects are also the subject of research. This paper undertakes to explore the literature about the connections between music education ...